SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.10 número2Caracterización molecular de las variedades de algodón nativo de color en la costa norte del PerúCuantificación de polifenoles totales y capacidad antioxidante en cáscara y semilla de cacao (Theobroma cacao L.), tuna (Opuntia ficus indica Mill), uva (Vitis Vinífera) y uvilla (Pourouma cecropiifolia) índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

  • No hay articulos citadosCitado por SciELO

Links relacionados

  • No hay articulos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Scientia Agropecuaria

versión impresa ISSN 2077-9917

Scientia Agropecuaria vol.10 no.2 Trujillo abr./jun. 2019

http://dx.doi.org/10.17268/sci.agropecu.2019.02.01 

ARTÍCULOS ORIGINALES

Effect of smoking and fat content on the dynamic perception of bacon by Brazilian consumers

 

Erick Saldaña1 ORCID iD https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4018-2852; Mariana Marinho Martins1; Beatriz Schmidt Menegali1 ORCID iD https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1153-4564; Miriam Mabel Selani2 ORCID iD https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8184-742X; Carmen J. Contreras-Castillo1,* ORCID iD https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0554-4694

1 Universidade de São Paulo (USP), Escola Superior de Agricultura "Luiz de Queiroz" (ESALQ), Departamento de Agroindústria, Alimentos e Nutrição (LAN), Piracicaba, SP 13418-900, Brazil.

2 Universidade Federal de São Carlos Campus Lagoa do Sino, Centro de Ciências da Natureza, Buri, SP, Brazil.


Abstract

The high heterogeneity of meat and meat products can influence the sensory perception of the consumers, being necessary to measure the real impact of these non-controllable variables on the dynamic sensory properties of the smoked bacon during tasting. The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of smoking and fat content on the dynamic sensory perception of bacon by Brazilian consumers. For this, four different samples were prepared, varying the fat content (from "low" to "high") and the type of smoking shown on the product label (natural and artificial). Thirty-five habitual smoked bacon consumers evaluated the dynamic sensory properties of the product using the Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS) method. The results were submitted to univariate (bandplot by attribute) and multivariate analyzes (Principal Component Analysis) to study both the dominance rate and its duration, respectively. The main attributes modified during tasting were "salty," "fatty," and "succulent," thus showing that the dynamic sensory perception of smoked bacon was modified by the type of smoking and the fat content of the sample.

Keywords: Sensory properties; Temporal Dominance of Sensations; Brazilian consumers.


1. Introduction

According to the annual report of the Brazilian Association of Animal Proteins of 2016, 89% of the pork meat produced in Brazil was destined to the production of industrialized products, and among them, the bacon stands out (Saldaña et al., 2019). Therefore, this product is noteworthy from a sensometric perspective, since any change in its composition and/or processing will modify its sensory properties, which will reflect on product sales (Saldaña et al., 2018a).

The sensory properties of meat products are very important intrinsic factors, since they are responsible for the formation of preference and, consequently, for consumers’ purchase intention (Tuorila and Monteleone, 2009). Descriptive analysis is the most sophisticated method for the quantitative description of the sensory properties of food, allowing to obtain the sensory profile of the product (Alcantara and Freitas-Sá, 2018). The sensory profile obtained by a well-trained assessor’s panel depends mainly on the products and the assessors. Commonly, the products are subjected to different treatments using a certain statistical design, for example "to reduce the content of sodium chloride of salamis, a randomized block design was used" (De Almeida et al., 2016) and "to modify the content of fat and salt in cooked bologna type sausages, a factorial design was used" (Ventanas et al., 2010). However, it is not always possible to develop a homogeneous product within the same treatment, as there are factors that we cannot control as we would like. In this regard, Bavay et al. (2014) demonstrated the inherent heterogeneity within the same variety of apples. In an analogous way to products of plant origin, products of animal origin are subject to variations in the proportion of fat and meat. Bacon is a product in which this characteristic is intensified, since the proportion of meat and fat in the pork belly is very variable. This heterogeneity can cause changes in sensory properties related to texture, aroma and taste. For example, within the same treatment, a high-fat bacon smoked with Eucalyptus wood, may have a lower salt taste intensity, since fat is usually a barrier to rapid dissolution of salt in saliva. In this sense, the sensory profile of a meat product not only depends on the type of smoking (Saldaña et al., 2018b) but also on the fat content (Lorido et al., 2015). However, few researchers have addressed the impact of these variables on the dynamic sensory profile of a product. The sensory changes due to different smoking process, as well as the variations in the meat/fat ratio are even more evident when studying the dynamic sensory properties of a meat product, which involve chewing and flavor release (Galmarini et al., 2016). In addition, the dynamic sensory methodologies are considered more realistic due to the possibility of studying the evolution of the sensory characteristics during tasting. The past decade has witnessed the development of different dynamic sensory methodologies to investigate changes in products over time. Among them, the Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS) has become fairly popular (Schlich, 2017). TDS is a dynamic multi-attribute methodology used to evaluate the temporal perception of dominant attributes of a product during consumption (Pineau et al., 2009). This methodology consists in the selection of dominant attributes by the consumers (an attribute is automatically deselected upon selection of another one) until all the sensations end.

Considering this panorama, the aim of this work was to evaluate the impact of the type of smoking and fat content on the dynamic sensory perception of bacon by Brazilian consumers using TDS.

2. Materials and methods

1.1. Samples

Two commercial bacons were purchased in a local market (São Paulo – Brazil), based on the smoking claim of the product label: a naturally and an artificially smoked product. Two meat/fat ratios were obtained for each smoking claim, summarizing 4 samples as shown in Figure 1.

1.2. Dynamic sensory evaluation

TDS test was performed in standardized sensory booths of the Escola Superior de Agricultura "Luiz de Queiroz" (ESALQ) / Universidade de São Paulo (USP). Samples were cut into cubes of approximately 2.5 g, cooked in a hot plate (200 °C) for five minutes, subsequently served monadically in disposable plastic plates coded with three-digit random numbers following a William’s Latin Square design to avoid biases. The data were collected using SensoMaker software (Pinheiro et al., 2013).

1.2.1. Attributes

The selected attributes (Table 1), based on previous studies (Saldaña et al., 2018b, 2019), were presented to the consumers according to Williams design to avoid biases, but each consumer received the same presentation order to facilitate attribute location (Merlo et al., 2018).

1.2.2. Participants

A total of 45 consumers were recruited at ESALQ/USP according to their frequency of bacon consumption (at least once a month), as well as their motivation and availability of time to participate in the familiarization and formal evaluation. However, only 35 consumers (18 59 years old) performed the test adequately. This amount of participants still represents a number higher than the minimum recommended (Pineau and Schilch, 2015).

1.2.3. Familiarization

A 30-minute familiarization session consisted of an introduction to sensory analysis of foods, followed by a presentation of the TDS test to consumers.

A strawberry-flavored chocolate was used to help consumers to fix the concept of dominant sensation, defined as "the sensation that catches/triggers your attention at a given time, while testing a product" (Pineau et al., 2009). In addition, a previous test was done with a sample of cereal bar prior to performing the formal evaluation, to make sure that the consumers understood the test.

1.2.4. Evaluation Protocol

Attributes were presented simultaneously on the computer screen (Figure 2). Then, the participants were instructed to start the test clicking on the "start" button and to choose the dominant sensation. Participants could select as many dominant sensations as they wanted or never select a sensation. After 60 seconds the test was finished. Water and bread were used for rinsing between bacon samples.

1.3. Data analysis

Data analysis was performed using TimeSense software (INRA, Dijon, France). Time was standardized between 0 (start of the test) and 1 (60 seconds) to align the tasting duration, allowing the comparison of the individual results. A bandplot by descriptor was used to represent the sequence of dominant sensations in each sample. The height of the bars corresponded to the dominance rate of the sample (Galmarini et al., 2017). In addition, the standardized duration of the dominances was represented by a biplot Principal Component Analysis (PCA), based on a covariance matrix (Thomas et al., 2016).

3. Results and discussion

Different dominant attributes during tasting (different sequence and dominance rate) were observed (Figure 3), indicating that the dynamic sensory profile of bacon samples was affected by the smoking method and the meat/fat content.

Bacons naturally smoked presented differrences in the salty flavor, smoky aftertaste, and stringy attributes due to differences in the proportion of fat. Interestingly, there was no noticeable change in the fatty attribute. This is probably because, at the time of cooking, a large part of the fat content was melted, reducing the changes in the perception of the fatty attribute. For the "artificial" samples, the fatty, succulent, salty, smoky and salty aftertaste attributes presented differences in the sequence and dominance rate due to the fat content. At the beginning of the tasting, the artificialM (artificial sample with the highest fat content) was perceived as fatty and succulent, while the artificialL (artificial sample with lower fat content) was perceived as smoky and salty. From half of the tasting onwards, the artificialL presented the highest rate and duration for the salty aftertaste and smoky aftertaste attributes. It is clear, then, that there is an interaction between the fat content and the type of smoking in bacons. In artificial smoking, differences in the fat/meat content of the bacons are even more pronounced. Once the effect of the type of smoking and the meat/fat ratio on the dynamic sensory perception of consumers has been demonstrated, it is necessary to study the standardized duration of the dominance. In Figure 4, the four samples and the eight attributes are displayed from a multivariate perspective. The confidence ellipses indicate whether two samples are similar (overlapping) or not (not overlapping).

First, the standardized duration of the succulent and fatty attributes was similar, since both vectors presented the same module and direction. These attributes were associated with samples with high fat content, both for the artificial and natural smoking process. This result was expected since they had a higher fat content. On the other hand, salty and smoky aftertaste attributes are opposed to succulent and fatty and, at the same time, are associated with products with less fat content. This behavior may be due to the fact that bacons with higher fat content leave a fatty mouth coating during product chewing, reducing the contact of the salt crystals with the saliva, decreasing dissolution and, therefore, the perception of saltiness (Chabanet et al., 2013; Lorido et al., 2015). The smoky aftertaste attribute, which characterizes the samples with low fat content, presented a behavior similar to the salty attribute. Probably the same phenomenon described in the salt particles occurred, i.e., fat prevented the molecules associated with the smoky aftertaste attribute from being easily released from the fat matrix, and considering that the chewing process lasts a few seconds, this sensation lasted a short time (Ventanas et al., 2010). The smoky and salty aftertaste attributes had an intermediate importance, varying only in the second dimension, which explained 17.3% of the variance of the data. Finally, the crunchy and stringy attributes were poorly represented by the first two dimensions; therefore, they did not have great relevance in the present study.

The evidence from this study suggests that standardizing the meat/fat content is essential for the study of the dynamic sensory properties of meat products.

4. Conclusions

The type of smoking and the fat content modified the dynamic sensory perception of bacons. Samples with high fat content had higher rate and duration of dominance for the fatty and succulent attributes, whereas for the low-fat samples these characteristics were observed for the salty attribute. The type of smoking also modified the sensory dynamics, without a clear pattern of increase or decrease of specific attributes.

Acknowledgments

E. Saldaña received the support of the "Consejo Nacional de Ciencia, Tecnología e Innovación Tecnológica CONCYTEC" from Peru (CIENCIACTIVA programme, PhD scholarship contract agreement No. 104-2016-FONDECYT). M. M. Martins and B. S. Menegali are thankful to "Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior" (CAPES) for the MSc scholarship.

 

References

Alcantara, M.; Freitas-Sá, D.D.G.C. 2018. Metodologias sensoriais descritivas mais rápidas e versáteis – uma atualidade na ciência sensorial. Brazilian Journal of Food Technology 21: 1–12.         [ Links ]

Bavay, C.; Bruun-Brockhoff, P.; Kuznetsova, A.; Maître, I.; Mehinagic, E.; Symoneaux, R. 2014. Consideration of sample heterogeneity and in-depth analysis of individual differences in sensory analysis. Food Quality and Preference 32: 126–131.         [ Links ]

Chabanet, C.; Tarrega, A.; Septier, C.; Siret, F.; Salles, C. 2013. Fat and salt contents affect the in-mouth temporal sodium release and saltiness perception of chicken sausages. Meat Science 94(2): 253-261.         [ Links ]

De Almeida, M.A.; Montes-Villanueva, N.D.; da Silva-Pinto, J.S.; Saldaña, E.; ContrerasCastillo, C.J. 2016. Sensory and physicochemical characteristics of low sodium salami. Scientia Agricola 73(4): 347-355.         [ Links ]

Galmarini, M.V.; Loiseau, A.-L.; Visalli, M.; Schlich, P. 2016. Use of Multi-Intake Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS) to Evaluate the Influence of Cheese on Wine Perception. Journal of Food Science 81: S2566–S2577.         [ Links ]

Galmarini, M.V.; Visalli, M.; Schlich, P. 2017. Advances in representation and analy-sis of mono and multi-intake Temporal Dominance of Sensations data. Food Quality and Preference 56: 247–255.         [ Links ]

Lorido, L.; Estévez, M.; Ventanas, J.; Ventanas, S. 2015. Salt and intramuscular fat modulate dynamic perception of flavour and texture in dry-cured hams. Meat Science 107: 39-48.         [ Links ]

Merlo, T.C.; Soletti, I.; Saldaña, E.; Schmidt, B.; Marinho, M.; Bortoluzzi, A.C.; Harada-Padermo, S. dos S.; Dargelio, M.D.B.; ContrerasCastillo, C.J. 2018. Measuring dynamics of emotions evoked by the packaging colour of hamburgers using Temporal Dominance of Emotions (TDE). Food Research International (in press).         [ Links ]

Pineau, N.; Schlich, P.; Cordelle, S.; Mathonnière, C.; Issanchou, S.; Imbert, A.; Rogeaux, M.; Etiévant, P.; Köster, E. 2009. Temporal Dominance of Sensations: Construction of the TDS curves and comparison with timeintensity. Food Quality and Preference 20(6): 450–455.         [ Links ]

Pineau, N.; Schilch, P. 2015. Temporal dominance of sensations (TDS) as a sensory profiling technique. In: Delarue, J.; Lawlor, J. Ben; Rogeaux, M.B.T.-R. S.P.T. (Eds.). Rapid Sensory Profiling Techniques: Applications in New Product Development and Consumer Research. [s.l.] Woodhead Publishing, 2015. p 269–306.         [ Links ]

Pinheiro, A.C.M.; Nunes, C. A.; Vietoris, V. 2013. SensoMaker: a tool for sensorial characterization of food products. Ciência e Agrotecnologia 37: 199–201.         [ Links ]

Saldaña, E. de Oliveira, A.; Selani, M.M.; Haguiwara, M.M.H.; Aurelio de Almeida, M.; Siche, R.; Contreras-Castillo, C.J. 2018a. A sensometric approach to the development of mortadella with healthier fats. Meat Science 137: 176–190.         [ Links ]

Saldaña, E.; Saldarriaga, L.; Cabrera, J.; Siche, R.; Aurélio de Almeida, M.; Behrens, J.C.; Selani, M.M.; Contreras-Castillo, C.J. 2018b. Descriptive analysis of bacon smoked with Brazilian woods from reforestation: methodological aspects, statistical analysis, and study of sensory characteristics. Meat Science 140: 44–50.         [ Links ]

Saldaña, E.; Saldarriaga, L.; Cabrera, J.; Behrens, J.H.; Selani, M.M.; Rios-Mera, J.; Contreras-Castillo, C.J. 2019. Descriptive and hedonic sensory perception of Brazilian consumers for smoked bacon. Meat Science 147: 60–69.         [ Links ]

Schlich, P. 2017. Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS): a new deal for temporal sensory analysis. Current Opinion in Food Science 15: 38–42.         [ Links ]

Thomas, A.; van der Stelt, A.J.; Prokop, J.; Lawlor, J.B.; Schlich, P. 2016. Alternating temporal dominance of sensations and liking scales during the intake of a full portion of an oral nutritional supplement. Food Quality and Preference 53: 159-167.         [ Links ]

Tuorila, H.; Monteleone, E. 2009. Sensory food science in the changing society: Opportunities, needs, and challenges. Trends in Food Science and Technology 20(2): 59-62.         [ Links ]

Ventanas, S.; Puolanne, E.; Tuorila, H. 2010. Temporal changes of flavour and texture in cooked bologna type sausages as affected by fat and salt content. Meat Science 85(3): 410419.         [ Links ]

 

Corresponding author
E-mail:
ccastill@usp.br (C. Contreras-Castillo)

 

Received February 11, 2019.
Accepted
March 17, 2019.

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons